Introductions
Shellshock and Post-Traumatic Stress
Disorder (PTSD) are two well-known conditions which can occur during and/or
after war trauma. Trauma is “a kind of wound” which originally referred to
physical injury but has become attributed to psychological wounds over the past
century (Garland, 1998: 9). Definitively, Shellshock is a nervous reaction in
warfare significant enough to stop normal human functioning, coined by Dr
Charles Myers during the First World War (WW1) (Shepherd, 2002 and Green,
2015). PTSD, included in ‘the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders’ (the DSM), from 1980, is when debilitating symptoms appear after a
soldier has seen horrific events, usually months or years later (Herzog, 2014:
128-129). Typically, both lead to long-lasting influences on the lives of the
individuals affected. They can be compared and contrasted in numerous ways; in
terms of historical origins, symptoms and causes, treatments, and criticisms
against them, all of which will be covered in this essay. Additionally, this is
an important aspect to investigate as it covers a gap in the research seldom
referred to in other works on the subject, providing a deeper understanding of
each condition and war
trauma in
general. Thus, despite space constraints, this essay aims to provide a concise
comparison of the main points of Shellshock and PTSD, exploring how each is
understood and dealt with, and concluding on whether or not they are the same
disorder.
The Historical
Context
A brief historical context is important
because it provides some grounding for comparisons in the themes that will
follow. Shellshock is considered the first partial acknowledgement of the
psychological costs of warfare, whereas the dawn of PTSD is thought to signify
the moment when veteran’s suffering was fully realised and put into law
(Wessely, 2006: 269). Each condition was formed in different environmental,
social and medical contexts. For example, WW1 was an industrialised war,
subjecting troops to static trench combat with shells constantly raining down
on them, so the first physical and later psychological understandings of
Shellshock developed in this context (Shepherd, 2002). Vietnam, however, was a
long guerrilla war fought in hot and wet jungle conditions where the enemy was
often unseen, so had immediately more psychological connotations (Shepherd,
2002). Additionally, psychiatry was in its infancy at the time of WW1, whereas
by Vietnam, although PTSD was not yet official, military psychiatry was a more
powerful force (Shepherd, 2002: 341).
During WW1, German Psychiatrists thought
Shellshock was not a condition, but instead showed that the affected soldier
was weak or lying, it was part of the “malingerer’s charter” for a free war
pension (Wessely, 2006: 271). The British had similar general views, believing
soldiers who succumbed lacked the “moral fibre” to keep fighting: thus,
psychiatric and medical diagnoses were avoided to reduce manpower wastage;
Shellshock was nothing more than a cowardly excuse (Wessely, 2006: 271 and
Shepherd, 2002). This was powered by Edwardian English and German traditional
values, in which manliness, self-control and patriotism were paramount
(Shepherd, 2002: 19). In complete contrast, PTSD is far more sympathetic and
political: it developed after the Holocaust and Vietnam, enthused by
psychiatrists like Robert J Lifton, Holocaust experts, Hiroshima survivors,
Vietnam Veterans Against the War (VVAW) and many more (Herzog, 2014: 150). They
campaigned for decades to obtain medical and social acknowledgement of
psychologically wounded victims of wars and other traumatic events, pushing
PTSD into the DSM and showing the public that trauma can be psychological, and
thus exist without visible signs (Herzog, 2014: 150-152 & Shepherd, 2002:
366-367). Differing from Shellshock, PTSD was born out of massive social
changes during the 1960s and 70s, including the civil rights movement, which
led to changes in outlooks on trauma, so it was never limited by tradition
(Shepherd, 2002). These historical understandings and ways of handling
Shellshock and PTSD suggest they are not merely dissimilar ways of looking at
the same condition, but that they are different conditions altogether.
The Symptoms and
Causes
The many symptoms and causes of each
condition are the main way to compare and contrast Shellshock and PTSD,
revealing much about how they are understood. The first recorded case of
Shellshock was a young soldier in 1914 who was almost killed by German
artillery, immediately believing he was going blind despite the absence of
physical injury (Shepherd, 2002: 1). He was seen by Charles Myers who coined
the term Shellshock when others started coming down with symptoms, including
the inability to smell, taste, hear, stand up or defecate properly, involuntary
movements and vomiting, amnesia, nightmares, odd gaits and so on (Shepherd,
2002: 1, 73-74). Barry Heard, an Australian Vietnam veteran who suffered from
PTSD years after the war, had comparable symptoms, including soiling himself in
everyday situations, shaking and weeping uncontrollably, and amnesia- he could
not remember the first months of his collapse, suggesting that they are similar
disorders (Heard, 2008: 265-267, 263-264). One officer in WW1 had recurring
nightmares of his mangled friend walking toward him; Heard also had frightening
dreams, eventually collapsing with severe PTSD after a nightmare during which
he returned to the Vietnamese jungle, causing symptoms of a severe heart attack
(Rivers, 1920: 190-191 and Heard, 2008: 261-264). Here it looks as though they
are comparable conditions, yet, conversely, PTSD usually develops months or
even decades later, as in Heard’s case, it has a “delayed onset,” whilst
Shellshock symptoms can have immediate effects (Heard, 2008 and Young, 1995:
107-108).
Additionally, there are other differences,
namely that accounts of Shellshock do not directly include the uncontrollable
aggression and guilt that can be symptoms of PTSD. For instance, AJ, an
ex-Royal Marine sniper suffering from PTSD after the war in Afghanistan, lost
control when another car cut him off; chasing the driver and then getting out
in the middle of the road shouting and swearing, despite the presence of his
family (Green, 2015: 5). Furthermore, Heard felt incredibly guilty for making a
mistake with his radio which meant some of his friends died before the medivac
helicopter could save them, the situation he returned to in the nightmare
(2008: 278-280). This guilt was arguably one of the causes of his symptoms, he
could not let go of his error during the traumatic events of that day in
Vietnam (Heard, 2008: 278-280). The distressing incident itself is the key
causal understanding of PTSD, part of criterion A in the DSM-IV-TR, the fourth
edition of the ‘Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,’
published in 2000 by the American Psychiatric Association, and providing global
standard criteria for classifying mental disorders. (Hunt, 2010: 53). It is
psychosomatic, the “traumatic memory” of the frightening and unforgettable
event leads to a malfunction in the mind’s ability to handle stress, causing
mental and physical symptoms (Hunt, 2010: 53 and Shepherd, 2002: 389). This
memory is permanently etched into the mind, hence the reason why AJ cannot
forget the faces of two young Afghan police who bled to death during a
firefight (Green, 2015: 1).
In opposition, Shellshock symptoms were
initially thought to be caused by somatic damage to the nervous system
resulting from shell blasts (Shepherd, 2002: 2-3). This is similar to the idea
that Mild Traumatic Brain Injuries (MTBI’s- concussions), which could happen
after large explosions, might be a cause of PTSD: people with these injuries
can suffer analogous symptoms, which led to Pentagon funded research (Green,
2015: 8). Otherwise, PTSD, distinct from Shellshock, is understood to be purely
psychologically produced. Yet, understandings of Shellshock did shift to
psychological ones, but the conventional view still differed from PTSD. For
instance, connected to the historical perspectives on moral causes, prevalent
military psychiatrists like Edgar Douglas Adrian and Lewis Yealland considered
Shellshock to be the reaction of cowards, caused by “a weakness of the will…and
the intellect, hyper-suggestibility and negativism” (Shepherd, 2002: 76). This
is the understanding that Shellshock victims have fixed ideas which override
the more positive suggestions of others, the resulting debilitating symptoms
being a way to avoid the front (Shepherd, 2002: 76). Another perspective contrasting
PTSD was based on the hereditary and personal predispositions of the soldier
rather than what he witnessed; some being more susceptible to Shellshock than
others (Young, 1995: 55). To illustrate, one account describes a soldier with
Shellshock symptoms, the cause being put down to his father being an alcoholic,
his excessive smoking habits- bringing in the moral element- and reports that
he was a nervous loner at school (Young, 1995: 55).
Nonetheless, despite being virtually
ignored and limited by the tradition-based military, some proposed the event
was the cause of Shellshock, the precursor for the understandings of PTSD
deliberated earlier. To illustrate, Rivers suggested symptoms are caused by
repressed traumatic memories of highly stressful events (1920: 186). He thought
that if these memories were repressed rather than properly faced, a behaviour
fortified by the stiff upper-lip culture of the time, symptoms would become
worse over the years (Rivers, 1920: 186). This parallels the delayed onset
characterising PTSD and Heard’s fear of being judged badly for his collapse,
which took decades of avoidance to become severe (2008: 263-267). Linking to
the politicised nature of PTSD, which contrasted Shellshock in the historical
context, enormous social changes, alongside media coverage of the Vietnam War,
led many Americans to mistreat already culture shocked veterans on their
homecoming, worsening or triggering PTSD symptoms (Shepherd, 2002: 343-344,
358-359). Thus, Shellshock and PTSD have relatively similar symptoms, but
mostly differ around causal understandings, meaning they cannot be defined as
the same condition.
Ways of Treating Shellshock and PTSD
Treatments for each condition also
highlight comparisons and contrasts. Reflecting the historical context,
psychiatry was in its infancy during WW1, and so the military objective was to
maintain manpower against cowardice. This meant most Shellshock treatments were
primitive, pushing for quick recovery and immediate re-deployment (Shepherd,
2002). The average treatment for Shellshock was to tell the soldier there was
nothing wrong with him and allow a few days rest (Shepherd, 2002: 57). Others
were sent out to work on French farms for a month before returning to the front
(Shepherd, 2002: 60). Adrian and Yealland, introduced in the last theme, had
the quick fixes the military desired: they treated Shellshock by authoritative
suggestion with the aid of faradic electricity (Shepherd, 2002: 76). Thus
military discipline was brought into treatment: they would tell the Shellshock
sufferer that they would recover when commanded, and would electrocute them
until they obeyed, the voltage constantly increasing to excruciatingly painful
levels (Shepherd, 2002: 76-77). They believed soldiers could be ‘re-educated’ by
suggestion, overcoming their supposedly weak intellect and its irrational
reasoning, so they applied the same methods to every case (Shepherd, 2002: 77).
Advocates of this treatment, such as Dr Arthur Hurst, claimed to be able to
cure Shellshock in twenty-four hours (Shepherd, 2002: 79). Peer pressure was
another useful method at the time, especially as Shellshock was highly
stigmatised, leading soldiers to ignore their symptoms to avoid losing face
(Shepherd, 2002: 57). Conversely, the military and charities encourage those
with PTSD symptoms to come forward for treatment, differing from the subjective
assumptions around Shellshock treatments (Green, 2015: 6-7).
Contrasting against the harsh approaches to
Shellshock, PTSD treatments are gentle and advanced, including prescription
drugs, cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT), psychodynamics and so forth,
providing a gradual healing process rather than a short sharp fix (Shepherd,
2002 and Young, 1995). For example, Barry Heard joined a twelve month programme
for PTSD sufferers at the Heidelberg Repatriation Hospital in Melbourne (2008:
272). It included exercise, lectures about PTSD, yoga and meditation, music,
art, making friends with fellow victims, CBT and group therapy, all based on
the acknowledgement that it takes time to heal and that some wounds never will
(Heard, 2008: 272-278). In one of the group therapy sessions, Heard released
the painful guilt of the radio incident; the discussion with the therapist and
other sufferers helping him see the event in a fairer light, and to remember
that he was only twenty-one at the time (Heard, 2008: 278-280). Accordingly,
PTSD treatment involves a very understanding and compassionate environment, a
far cry from the lonely tortures Shellshock casualties faced in Yealland’s
company. CBT teaches strategies for dealing with anxiety and stress, replacing
negative thoughts with positive ones, in turn helping victims reinterpret
traumatic events (Young, 1995: 177-179). Heard was taught breathing and muscle
controlling techniques, the latter preventing him from soiling himself as often
(2008: 283-284).
The marine sniper AJ tried “Eye Movement
Desensitisation and Reprocessing” (EMDR) for his PTSD, which encourages
sufferers to recollect traumatic memories in order to desensitise/objectify the
emotions around them, which did not work for him (Green, 2015: 4). This
underscores the understanding around PTSD that each person is affected
differently, so particular combinations of treatments are tailored to the
individual through regular discussions with them about what is working and what
needs adjusting (Young, 1995: 179-186). Following on from Rivers understanding
of Shellshock causes conferred earlier, PTSD treatment is consequently grounded
in facing the traumatic event, contrasting the orthodox Shellshock ‘cures’
which ignored distressing memories in favour of overcoming reputed weaknesses:
a quick fix for military efficiency.
Despite the vast contrasts, as with causes,
PTSD treatments are somewhat similar to Rivers humane ones. As he understood
the event to be the cause of the symptoms, he talked with victims to try and
make them feel better about and accept what happened, to prevent unhealthy
repression (Rivers, 1920). Myers used hypnosis for the same reason, to gently help
Shellshock sufferers extricate themselves from the traumatic memory, and its
symptoms, by calmly reliving it, similar to Heard’s experience in group therapy
(Shepherd, 2002: 49). For example, returning to the Shell-shocked officer with
nightmares of his dead friend blown apart by a shell, Rivers highlighted the
fact that he likely died instantly without suffering, something the officer
took comfort in, leading him to find closure in a dream where he spoke to the
friend, his health subsequently returning (1920: 190-192). Still, treatments
like these were outweighed by the likes of Dr Gordon Holmes who thought
Shellshock should be callously cut out to stem the flow of hysteria through the
ranks, regardless of the underlying psychological causes (Shepherd, 2002:
48-49). Accordingly, the treatments contrast so much that it is impossible to
argue that Shellshock and PTSD are merely different ways of understanding and
handling the same condition.
The Criticisms of each Condition
Lastly some criticisms of Shellshock and
PTSD reveal comparisons and contrasts between them, and emphasise others
already mentioned. Although PTSD understandings and treatments are
advantageously more sympathetic than Shellshock, any trauma, not just war, can be
considered a cause, so someone can abuse it by faking symptoms (Shepherd, 2002
and Summerfield, 2001). It has become so wide ranging that it “lacks
specificity,” and risks becoming “clinically meaningless” as anything from
being mugged to childbirth and “verbal sexual harassment” are considered causes
of PTSD (Summerfield, 2001: 96-97). To illustrate, in the western materialistic
society an industry has formed around compensation claims in the UK and
elsewhere, so people seek PTSD status to make money, even for minor incidents
or normal job stress, one ambulance driver claiming £5000 because he saw people
dying at work (Summerfield, 2001: 96 and Toolis, 2009). From previous
discussion, this was never a problem with Shellshock as it was assumed those
with it were cowards or malingerers who wanted to avoid being sent back to
fight (Wessely, 2006: 271). Accordingly, the term PTSD can be considered too
inclusive whilst Shellshock was never wide-ranging enough.
Nevertheless, analogous to the official
understanding of Shellshock at the time, some believe PTSD is not a condition,
but just a lie used by malingerers, a view promulgated by publications such as
‘Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: How to Apply for 100 Percent Total Disability,’
which encouraged faking symptoms to receive benefits (Shepherd, 2002: 387 &
395). Also, as touched on in parts of this essay, both have stigma in common,
albeit on differing levels: Shellshock was stigmatised officially in the views
of generals and psychiatrists such as Yealland, and unofficially as the peer
pressure treatment emphasised (Shepherd, 2002). The stigma surrounding PTSD is
mostly unofficial, deterring many from receiving treatment. To illustrate,
Heard avoided his PTSD diagnosis because he feared being judged badly (2008:
267 and Green, 2015: 6). Clearly, with all these problems, a more specific
understanding of war trauma is required; PTSD may eventually be replaced by
something new, as occurred with Shellshock a century ago. Therefore, in terms
of criticisms, Shellshock and PTSD are products of different times with their
own problems, some of which are comparable, others contrasting. Despite a few
similarities, they cannot be argued to be the same condition here.
Conclusions
Shellshock and PTSD are both born of war
trauma, meaning that there are a few similarities, including Barry Heard’s
symptoms equating to Shellshock ones, the possibility of somatic causes, the
stigma around them, and the views of Rivers and Myers that symptoms come from
the traumatic event and require gentle treatments. Nevertheless, these are
eclipsed by the differences between the two conditions, taking into account
their separate historical contexts, the delayed onset of PTSD against the more
immediate Shellshock, the way conventional Shellshock understandings focus on
morality and predispositions whereas PTSD focuses on the traumatic event,
Shellshock lacking the politicisation of PTSD, the quick fix aim of primitive
Shellshock treatments versus the advanced healing process for PTSD victims, and
so on. Further analysis is required in order to fully examine the themes
discussed in this article, especially in terms of how each is diagnosed,
Freud’s views, and the many other thinkers that had to be missed out.
Nevertheless, this comparison has shown that “there continues to be no
consensus in the meaning of the story,” there are many different hypothetical
conditions which are all eventually replaced by something else, but war trauma
itself continues to exist (Herzog, 2014: 155). Overall, looking at the ways
Shellshock and Post-Traumatic stress disorder are understood and handled, the
similarities, mostly concentrated in symptoms and the views of William Rivers,
are heavily outweighed by the contrasts, so they cannot be considered the same
condition.
References
Freud, S. (2010)
Beyond the Pleasure Principle.
Translated from the German by J.
Strachey. USA: Pacific Publishing Studio.
Garland, C. (1998) ‘Thinking about Trauma’,
in
Understanding Trauma: A Psychoanalytic
Approach
. London: Karna, pp. 9-31.
Green, M. (2015) ‘Guilt-even innocent
guilt- is an evil thing: how soldiers struggle to cope when they come home’,
The Guardian
, 7 October. Available at:
http://www.theguardian.com/uk-news/2015/oct/07/guilt-even-innocent-guilt-evil-thing-soldiers-struggle-cope-when-come-home
(Accessed: 10 November 2015).
Heard, B. (2008)
Well Done, Those Men: Memoirs of a Vietnam Veteran
. Melbourne:
Scribe Publications.
Herzog, D. (2014) ‘The Obscenity of
Objectivity: Post-Holocaust Anti-Semitism and the Invention-Discovery of
Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder’, in Lebovic, N. and Killen, A. (eds.)
Catastrophes: A History of an Operative
Concept
. Oldenbourg: De Gruyter, pp. 128-155.
Hunt, N. (2010)
Memory, War and Trauma
. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Rivers, W. (1920) ‘The Repression of War
Experience’, in
Instinct and the
Unconscious: A Contribution to a Biological Theory of the Psycho-Neuroses
.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 185-198.
Shepherd, B. (2002)
A War of Nerves: Soldiers and Psychiatrists 1914-1994
. London:
Pimlico.
Summerfield, D. (2001) ‘The invention of
post-traumatic stress disorder and the social usefulness of a psychiatric
category’,
British Medical Journal
,
322, pp. 95-98.
Toolis, K. (2009) ‘The great trauma gold
rush: How 'post-traumatic stress disorder' has become the compensation
culture's new money-spinner’
, Daily Mail,
28 July. Available at: http://www.dailymail.co.uk/news/article-1202347/The-great-trauma-gold-rush-How-post-traumatic-stress-disorder-compensation-cultures-new-money-spinner.html
(Accessed: 12 November 2015).
Wessely, S. (2006) ‘Twentieth Century
Theories on Combat Motivation and Breakdown’,
Journal of Contemporary History
, 41(2), pp. 269-286.
Young, A. (1995)
The Harmony of Illusions: Inventing Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder
.
New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
Copyright statement
© Daniel Roberts. This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International Licence (CC BY).