Skip to main content
Emotional Intelligence—Fact or Fad?

Essay

Emotional Intelligence—Fact or Fad?

Author:

Abstract

This essay explores the concept of Emotional Intelligence (EI) and the debates surrounding its validity. Scholars argue that EI significantly impacts workplace, relationships, mental health, and daily decision-making. They advocate for its incorporation into formal and informal training programs. Critics raise concerns about its measurement, cross-cultural applicability, and the conceptual breadth of "emotional intelligence." The debates emphasise the need for a nuanced understanding of the construct, which will enable the clarification of a possible conflation with other personality traits or social skills. This essay concludes with evidence supporting the value of EI in various domains, stating that further research is essential to establish its psychometric credentials to gain broader acceptance among psychologists and establish its place in psychology.

Keywords: psychology, emotional intelligence, fact, fad, personality traits and relationship

How to Cite:

Chiadinotu, C. V., (2024) “Emotional Intelligence—Fact or Fad?”, Essex Student Journal 15(1). doi: https://doi.org/10.5526/esj.284

0498d558-c192-47bf-9288-91c818a56351

Introduction—Key Concepts

It is well recognised that the ability to manage emotions—whether one’s own or those of others—is crucial for optimal psychological and social functioning. Emotions are internal states that mediate physiological awareness and behavioural expressions of subjective feelings, including love, pleasure, jealousy, guilt, pain, etc. According to Lerner and Kelter (2000), people’s emotional states inform their level of decision-making and many other aspects of their lives. Intelligence represents a multifaceted and intricate construct, resistant to precise encapsulation within a singular definition. It is fundamentally referred to as the capacity to acquire knowledge, encompassing the comprehension of multifarious dimensions of intricate concepts and the adept navigation of complex problem-solving tasks (McGregor, 2007). In the discerning analysis of Gottfredson (1997), intelligence emerges as a robust and compelling determinant that significantly augments one's prospects for attaining educational, financial, and occupational success. Furthermore, intelligence plays an instrumental role in facilitating effective interpersonal interactions and socialisation skills.

What is Emotional Intelligence?

Emotional intelligence (EI) has been defined as the ability to notice one’s emotions and those of other people (Salovey and Mayer, 1990). Charles Darwin’s (1872) seminal work on emotional expression in animals and humans appears to have informed the concept of EI. The concept was popularised by Daniel Goleman, who classified EI into four broad components: self-awareness, self-management, empathy or social awareness, and social skills. Goleman emphasised that EI was more important than the intelligence quotient (IQ) for success in education, the workplace, and leadership roles (Goleman, 1998). According to Nelis et al. (2011), Goleman surveyed evidence indicating the strong role that the amygdala plays in emotional regulation. Their research states that EI can be improved through knowledge and exposure. Since introducing the concept of EI, many studies, debates, and criticisms have revolved around it and will be discussed and compared in this essay. Some researchers have taken it up and promoted it, while others have seen it as overhyped and possibly redundant. The rest of this short essay examines the arguments for and against EI as a psychological construct.

Emotional Intelligence is a Fact

According to Brackett et al. (2011), EI is an attribute that allows people to perform well in such diverse areas as the workplace, social relationships, academics, leadership, mental health, and daily decision-making. Hence, they argued that training on EI should be incorporated into formal settings, such as workplaces and academic institutions, as well as more relaxed programs, such as life coaching and meditation sessions (Goleman, 1995; Lopes et al., 2011). Similarly, Joseph and Newman (2010) conducted a meta-analysis of studies. They found that high levels of EI predicted sound mental health, high job performance, and strong leadership skills—key constituents of positive psychology (PP). Furthermore, the congruence between EI and PP principles implies that the principles of EI can be strategically employed to enrich the practice of PP. This convergence holds the potential to foster healthier interpersonal relationships and enhance overall life satisfaction for individuals (Salovey et al, 2009). Fredrickson’s (2004) “broaden-and-build” theory elucidates the attributes of positive emotions that can be effectively applied to augment the efficacy of EI as a facet of PP. Such integration promises to drive personal development to new heights and enhance social understanding in various contexts, including workplaces, educational institutions, and community gatherings.

Mikolajczak and Luminet (2008) argued that EI encourages self-efficacy, equipping people to effectively deal with difficult life situations, and protects against burnout (Lopes et al., 2011). Along the same line, Reuven Bar-On (2006) asserted that successfully managing environmental needs and pressures depends on the sense of “interrelated emotional and social competencies” that help us understand ourselves and others. He divided these competencies into five categories: intrapersonal, interpersonal, adaptability, stress management, and general mood. Each of these aspects, he suggested, would be well-developed in a person with high “emotional-social intelligence” (Bar-On, 2006). Researchers have argued that people who show competence in managing their own emotions appear to be adept at recognising and dealing with the emotions of others (Downey et al., 2008; Maltby et al., 2010). In sum, the studies outlined above present empirical evidence for EI as a predictor of outstanding performance in various areas of life. Through the definitions of EI, other aspects of psychology, such as social, PP, and similar others, could flourish more, which presents EI as being a fact (Bar-On, 2010). Nevertheless, several psychologists have challenged the conceptual basis of the construct.

Emotional Intelligence is a Fad

It is interesting to note that a prominent psychologist, Hans Eysenck, found fault with the concept of EI. In his view, emotional “intelligence” was not nearly as scientifically testable as IQ, nor was there a clear benchmark by which to compare EI across individuals (Eysenck, 2018). Van der Zee et al. (2002) also disputed the claim that EI predicts job performance better than IQ, indicating that the reverse accorded more with the facts. On a more conceptual level, some researchers have raised concerns about whether instruments purporting to measure EI are measuring something else. For instance, Brackett and Salvey (2006) cautioned that researchers trying to measure EI may inadvertently include other personality traits (such as the Big Five trait, agreeableness) or even general intelligence. They therefore tried to point to the fact that EI cannot be measured without other things interfering and affecting its true result.

Similarly, Mayer et al. (2008) suggested that what some people call EI could be no more than a bundle of other distinct personality traits and social skills, such as empathy and self-awareness. Apart from questioning the validity of the construct, Cote and Miners (2006) observed that the concept of EI has limited applicability to other societies and cultures apart from Western ones. Finally, researchers such as Matthews et al. (2004) have claimed that EI, as usually defined, is a concept too broad to be precisely measured given the two significant words that make it up. According to Girrell (2005), what is referred to as EI would be better off addressed as “emotional intimacy”. For him, an individual needs to have some level of experience with what others go through before they can fully understand and know how to manage such situations. Locke (2005) argued that EI is a wrong concept; for him, it has no connection to the actual meaning of “intelligence”, and he perceived the use of EI to evaluate competence for leadership and job qualification as a biased measure.

Conclusion

The debate surrounding EI is multidimensional and cuts across its perceived values and criticism within psychological domains. While proponents highlight its potential to significantly impact various aspects of life, including workplace performance, social relationships, and mental health, critics have raised valid concerns regarding its measurement, cross-cultural applicability, and conceptual clarity. Despite the considerable research efforts dedicated to applying EI in diverse social settings, there remains an essential need for more rigorous research and investigation into its psychometric foundations. Without establishing robust psychometric credentials, it is unlikely that EI will achieve the level of near-unanimous acceptance among psychologists that IQ has garnered. Therefore, while acknowledging the potential benefits of EI, further research is imperative to address the existing concerns and clarify its conceptual boundaries. Only through continued exploration and refinement can EI truly find its place within the broader landscape of psychology.

References

Bar-On, R. (2010) ‘Emotional intelligence: An integral part of positive psychology’, South African Journal of Psychology40(1), pp. 54­–62. Available at: https://hdl.handle.net/10520/EJC98569

Brackett, M. A. and Salovey, P. (2006) ‘Measuring emotional intelligence with the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT)’, Psicothema, 18, pp. 34­–41. PMID: 17295955

Brackett, M. A., Rivers, S. E., and Salovey, P. (2011) ‘Emotional intelligence: Implications for personal, social, academic, and workplace success’, Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5(1), pp. 88­–103. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00334.x

Darwin, C. (1965) ‘The expression of the emotions in man and animals (Vol. 526)’, Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published in 1872). PMID: 19203144. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013386

Cote, S. and Miners, C. T. H. (2006) ‘Emotional intelligence, cognitive intelligence, and job performance’, Administrative Science Quarterly, 51(1), pp. 1­–28. Available at: https://doi.org/10.2189/asqu.51.1.1

Eysenck, H. J. (2018) Intelligence: A new look. Transaction Publishers. ISBN 13: 978-0-7658-0707-6. Available at: https://doi.org/10.4324/9781351310048

Fredrickson, B. L. (2004) ‘The broaden–and–build theory of positive emotions’, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences359(1449), pp. 1367­–1377. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2004.1512

Goleman, D. (1995) ‘Emotional intelligence: Why it can matter more than IQ’, Bantam Books.

Goleman, D. (1998) ‘Working with emotional intelligence’, Bantam Books. PMID: 10177113

Gottfredson, L. S. (1997) ‘Mainstream science on intelligence: An editorial with 52 signatories, history, and bibliography’, Intelligence, 24(1), pp. 13­–23.

Joseph, D. L. and Newman, D. A. (2010) ‘Emotional intelligence: An integrative meta-analysis and cascading model’, Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(1), pp. 54­–78. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017286

Lerner, J. S. and Keltner, D. (2000) ‘Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice’, Cognition and Emotion, 14(4), pp. 473­–493. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1080/026999300402763

Locke, E.A. (2005) ‘Why emotional intelligence is an invalid concept’, Journal of Organizational Behavior26(4), pp. 425­–431. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1002/job.318

Lopes, P. N., Salovey, P., Cote, S., and Beers, M. (2011) ‘Emotion regulation abilities and the quality of social interaction’, Emotion, 11(5), 1010­–1020. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.113

Maltby, J., Day, L., and Macaskill, A. (2010) Personality, individual differences and intelligence. Pearson Education.

Mathews, G., Zeidner, M., and Roberts, R. D. (2004) Emotional intelligence: Science and myth. MIT Press.

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., and Caruso, D. R. (2008) ‘Emotional intelligence: new ability or eclectic traits?’, American Psychologist, 63(6), pp. 503­–517. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.63.6.503

McGregor, D. (2007) ‘Developing thinking; developing learning’ McGraw-Hill Education (UK).

Mikolajczak, M. and Luminet, O. (2008) ‘Trait emotional intelligence and the cognitive appraisal of stressful events: An exploratory study’, Personality and Individual Differences, 44(7), pp. 1445­–1453. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.12.012

Nelis, D., Quoidbach, J., Mikolajczak, M., and Hansenne, M. (2009) ‘Increasing emotional intelligence: (How) is it possible?’, Personality and Individual Differences47(1), pp. 36­–41. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.01.046

Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D., and Yoo, S. H. (2009) ‘The positive psychology of emotional intelligence’. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195187243.013.0022

Brody, N. (2005) ‘What cognitive intelligence is and what emotional intelligence is not?’, Psychological Inquiry, 15, pp. 234­–238. Available at: https://www.jstor.org/stable/20447233

Locke, E. (2005) ‘Why emotional intelligence is an invalid concept’, Journal of Organisational Behaviour, 26, pp. 425­–431. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1002/job.318

©Chiamaka Vivian Chiadinotu. This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International Licence (CC BY).

Share

Downloads

Information

Metrics

  • Views: 598
  • Downloads: 188

Citation

Download RIS Download BibTeX

File Checksums

(MD5)
  • PDF: 477e0cf337497766a4d7178c0972c0a7
  • Word: fb8a439698764e002360850e1d70d4fe
  • HTML: 366c2244dd40032f1ae7d86acde07c7b

Table of Contents