An accumulation of evidence exists to
understand why there are gender differences in regard to being more risk-averse
and competitive. This disparity is further apparent when we attempt to
understand the impact it has on women, with respect to the gender gaps in wages
and gender discrimination in job roles in the labour market. As well as,
understanding whether women are more risk-averse when applying for more
competitive jobs in fields that are male dominated. The involvement of women in
the labour market has been a prolonged discussion, however, since the last few
decades, women have been seen to slowly disengage from their childbearing role
and are paid for being legally employed. Therefore, with this in mind, I will
be exploring whether gender differences in taste and competition in particular;
has led to the growing gap between men and women in the labour market.
Gender differences in labour markets exists
in numerous ways, one way is via preference of the job field and the pay grade.
When determining why there is an ongoing gap between the wages paid to men and
women, we find that this relates to ‘women’s greater distaste for competition’
(Kleinjans, 2009). However, we cannot simply confine it to the lack of
competitiveness in women as the sole reason for the gap. Previously, women have
known to be more risk-averse than men when making decisions for themselves and
this has contributed to the gap. For instance, there are fewer women employed
in senior roles than men due to the lack of representation of women. For
example, many corporate companies are insistent that more women take on senior
roles as there is a huge gap in the hierarchy. Similarly, caring
responsibilities are often taken by women which also explains the reason for
choosing part – time jobs. Finally, women incline to work in less competitive
job fields such as carers or as hairstylists as such jobs provide more
flexibility, and these jobs are less competitive as opposed to roles such as civil
engineering.
Therefore, amongst many reasons for the
disparity in gender differences, the lack of riskiness and non-negotiation
partly contributes to the gender gap. Despite the Equal Pay Act in 1970,
according to the UK statistics, in 2020 only ‘71.8% of females’ were employed
as opposed to ‘80.6%’ employed males. This gender differentiation in pay is
also apparent in other countries such as Pakistan, who have the world’s highest
gender pay gap of 36%. Based on the statistics exhibited here ‘the extent of
gender difference in risk - aversion’ can be a used as contributing factor when
understanding the ‘gender gaps in competitiveness’ (Clot, Giusta, and Razzu,
2020).
As mentioned earlier, evidence shows that
women are more risk-averse than men. In particular, married women with children
and single mothers with children. Job sectors such as engineering and manual
labour work consist of a more significant risk of death, and women especially
tend to steer away from such roles when they have a family. In support of this,
Sloane
and Grazier (2006) established comparable
results displaying a negative coefficient of -5.942 for women with children as
being more risk-averse than single fathers when using family structure as a
proxy. This could rationalise why women prefer to work in less injurious type
of jobs. Based on this, they ‘suggest that preferences for risk do affect
occupational choice in the UK’. Thus, being more risk-averse can lead to a
widening gap in the labour market, and this is more apparent in women.
Correspondingly, this is evident in Hakim (2015) work where she constructed an
explanation using ‘preference theory’ which clarifies how women associate
different values to each counterpart of their lifestyle by prioritising
responsibilities such as domestic roles over a higher paid unsafe role.
Another aspect to explain the gender
difference in risk and competition is the notion of how women are represented
in today’s society. Mass media is to blame for the challenges women have faced
in their day-to-day life. Not only has mass media negatively affected their
lifestyle but also the labour market. The stereotyped discrimination of women
in the media shown to be less equivalent than a man in male-dominated
environments, has altered women’s’ mindset in challenging themselves to endure
the risk and to apply for more male – dominated roles. The lack of female
representation has also been a key issue here and sociologists state how an
environment can influence this. An example of this is seen in (Booth and Nolen,
2012) experiment where they found ‘women in gender-segregated groups will be
more competitive and less risk-averse than in mixed-sex
groups’
.
This highlights how a higher representation of females in the same environment
has potential to close the gap as opposed to when there is a lack of females.
As initially stated, preferences when
exploring the job market is prevalent and in particular, to ensure the job itself
pays us well and matches our values. When choosing a job, we consciously seek
roles that meet our lifestyle. In this case, we can state that this is why
undoubtedly ‘some occupations are more attractive to women and others more
attractive to men’ (Cortes and Pan, 2017). However, besides lifestyle, every
decision we make as individuals’ routes back to the influence of psychological
reasoning as well. As mentioned earlier, women tend to look for less risky and
less competitive job roles as they are biologically made to care for their
children and therefore may not have the time and resources for a full-time job.
Thus, ‘occupational segregation’ enhances the gender gap in the labour market.
Alternatively, another angle on this debate
suggests that women are less willing to accept offers outside of their chosen
careers and their inability to do this also creates a distinctive gap between
men and women ‘reinforcing their poorer position in the labour market’ (Booth,
2009). Booth (2009) also highlights ‘if women do manage to obtain a position of
leadership they are then evaluated less favourably because they do not fit
society's prescriptions of what is appropriate behaviour for women’. The idea
of how women should be and should not be is society’s perception that has led
to the creation of the glass ceiling. This is supported by Beamen et al (2003),
as cited by Booth (2009) who found evidence that women who broke the glass
ceiling and took on senior positions were evaluated more harshly than their
‘male counterparts on many performance dimensions’. Therefore, reinforcing the
difficulties women still have in the labour market despite breaking society’s
barriers, justifying that there is still a widening gap between men and women
in the workforce.
In conjunction with the above, several
experiments have been conducted by economists and psychologists to understand
why there is a gender difference in taste for risk and competition. Azmat and Petrongolo
(2014), explore occupational segregation as mentioned earlier and its potential
input to the gap. Using laboratory experiments, they found ‘men are more
tolerant of risk than women’ which explains why ‘men thrive in competitive
environments’ which also clarifies why men find it easier to negotiate their
pay grades as opposed to women as they are represented well in ‘high – profile
careers’.
In contrast to this evidence, an
alternative laboratory experiment conducted by (Clot, Giusta and Razzu, 2020)
where they observed 115 professionals found results showing how women did not
‘shy away from competition and performed as well as men’. They did not find a
disparity in the performance between men and women. Women performed just as
well or better as men when they were competing against a random participant (no
disclosure of who they were against). They found a small gap in the entry of
the tournament but not once they were aware of the activity itself. Whilst this
juxtaposes the above claims that women are more risk-averse, there are
limitations to this study as it does not test their competitiveness in the
labour market but rather as a dependent variable in testing competitiveness
alone.
To summarise, there is distinctly a large
set of evidence to prove there are gender differences in taste for risk and
competition. Alongside external factors such as the influence of media and
‘psychological differences between men and women’. Men might also feel
threatened and may begin to question their masculinity when working alongside
female colleagues. This can cause women to become reluctant to take the risk
and to apply for senior positions generally dominated by men, which further
reinforces the existing gap in the labour market. To overcome the gender
differences in the workforce, many institutes are enforcing female students to
take on STEM subjects to break the glass ceiling.
1.
Azmat, G. and Petrongolo, B.,
(2014).
Gender and the labour market | VOX, CEPR Policy Portal
.
[online] Voxeu.org. Available at:
https://voxeu.org/article/gender-and-labour-market
[Accessed 10 March 2021]
2.
Booth, A. and Nolen, P., (2012).
Choosing to compete: How different are girls and
boys?.
Journal
of Economic Behavior & Organization
, [online] 81(2), pp.542-555.
Available at:
<https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0167268111002010>
[Accessed 11 March 2021]
3.
Booth, A., (2009). Gender and
competition.
Labour Economics
, [online] 16(6), pp.599-606.
Available at:
https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0927537109000906
[Accessed 10 March 2021]
4.
Clot, S., Giusta, M. and Razzu,
G., (2020).
Gender gaps in competition: new experimental evidence from
UK professionals
. [online] Reading.ac.uk. Available at:
http://www.reading.ac.uk/web/files/economics/emdp202015.pdf
[Accessed 10 March 2021]
5.
Cortes, P. and Pan, J.,
(2017).
Occupation and Gender
. [online] Iza.org. Available at:
https://www.iza.org/publications/dp/10672/occupation-and-gender
Accessed 10 March 2021]
6.
Grazier, S. and Sloane, P.,
(2006).
(PDF) Accident risk, gender, family status and occupational
choice in the UK
. [online] ResearchGate. Available at:
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/257377250_Accident_risk_gender_family_status_and_occupational_choice_in_the_UK
[Accessed 10 March 2021]
7.
Hakim, C., (2015).
Preference
Theory
. [online] Wiley Online Library. Available at:
https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/9781405165518.wbeosp138?saml_referrer
[Accessed 12 March 2021]
8.
Kleinjans, K., (2009). Do gender
differences in preferences for competition matter for occupational
expectations?
Journal of Economic Psychology
, [online] 30(5),
pp.701-710. Available at:
https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0167487009000269
[Accessed 17 March 2021]
9.
Reuben, E., Sapienza, P. and
Zingales, L., (2021).
Taste for competition and the gender gap among
young business professionals
. [online] Kellogg.northwestern.edu. Available
at:
https://www.kellogg.northwestern.edu/faculty/sapienza/htm/TasteCompetition.pdf
[Accessed 11 March 2021]
10.
Beaman,
L., Chattopadhyay, R.,
Duflo
, E.,
Pande
,
R. and
Topalova
, P., (2008).
Powerful Women:
Does Exposure Reduce
Bias?
.
[online] Economics.mit.edu. Available at:
https://economics.mit.edu/files/3122
[Accessed 12 March 2021]
©
Akshana Ravikumar.
This article
is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International Licence (CC
BY).